Dr. Gerald S. Hecht
Assistant Professor of Psychology
College of Sciences
webmaster@psiwebsubr.org
PSYC 381 - Sensation & Perception Exam 3 Study Guide

Vision Part II: From the Eye to the Brain
THE IMAGE DRAWN onto the retina by eye optics is transduced chemically in the rods and cones. This chemistry closes channels in their plasma membranes, and that increases their polarity. Their synapses onto bipolar cells slow or stop in their activity, and this releases bipolars  from inhibition. Both of these cells are also leaky (pacemakers), and they can activate their synapses--which are also inhibitory.
   The principal target of bipolars is ganglion cells, which have been pacing action potentials to higher brain centers in a disordered fashion. The activation of bipolars because appropriate light has struck their receptor cells--imposes inhibitory order onto this ganglion cell discharge, creating a rough image that is projected centrally. That image will be "fuzzy" in the case of rods because information from entire "gangs" of them communicate with a single bipolar cell--cones however produce crisp, clean sharp images stemming from their "direct line" to the bipolars (each cone gets to have "its own" bipolar cell).
Do I have to draw you a picture? Ok, here are two pictures--one of the rod situation and another of the cone situation: rodsimgconeimg
   This is the data of the optic nerve. And a nice summary (I think) of the material covered on the last exam.
Optic Tract and Chiasm
chiasmimg
GRANULE CELL AXONS converge from the entire retina on an area medial to the visual axis of each eye, and at this point they gain their myelin sheath (which would not have been transparent on the surface of the retina!) and exit the retina and the eyeball as the optic "nerve." In humans there are about 1,000,000 axons in each nerve, and these take up space. Which means that where they exit the eye, there is no room for retinal columns, and this creates a blind spot. Since the two spots in the two eyes do not coincide in their retinal fields, one eye sees what the other does not. You learn to ignore this hole in your vision.   The nerve passes through a tendonous ring, which is the origin of the recti muscles, and enters the cranial cavity via the orbital foramen. At this point the tough fibrous sheath of the nerve (which is continuous with the sclera) merges with the dura mater lining the cranial cavity. Beyond this point, the CNS affiliation of the tract is evident from the various meningeal relations (figure: "Optic Tract & Meninges). In its route, the tracts are still passing medially as well as posteriorly,menigesimg and the nerves of the two sides meet at the optic chiasm.
   In vertebrates with laterally-directed eyes, the nerves may cross entirely at this decussation, but in man, where the eyes have converged with much binocular overlap, only about half of these axons cross over. These visual fields were shown in detail in the description of retinal function (The last study guide). The effect of this mixed projection to the optic cortex is that the entire visual field (as opposed to retinal field) is projected to the contralateral (opposite side) cortex. If you observe carefully, you may see this. Often when you first awake, your eyes will have moved out of coherence, and when you first open them, one of the images will shift to match the other slowly enough for you to see it "snap" into place.
whichcrosses
Lateral Geniculate Body
The optic tract (as it is now called) enters the diencephalon and extends to the lateral geniculate nucleus ("G" on the image pathway drawing above).
The lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus is a part of the brain, which is the primary processor of visual information, received from the retina, in the CNS.

The LGN receives information directly from the retina, and sends projections directly to the primary visual cortex. In addition, it receives many strong feedback connections from the primary visual cortex.

Ganglion cells of the retina send axons to the LGN through the optic nerve. Although it is generally considered to be a cranial nerve, and is always listed as cranial nerve II, in reality the retina and optic nerve arise as an outpocketing of the developing diencephalon. Rather than a proper nerve, then, the optic nerve is really a tract of the brain.

The LGN is a distinctively layered structure ("geniculate" means "bent like a knee"). In most primates, including humans, it has six layers of cell bodies with layers of neuropil in between, in an arrangement something like a club sandwich or layer cake, with cell bodies of LGN neurons as the "cake" and neuropil as the "icing".

These six layers contain two types of cells. The cells in layers 1 and 2 are large, or magnocellular ; others in layers 3, 4, 5, and 6 are smaller, or parvocellular. (The Latin prefix "parvo-" means "small"; some authors prefer the term parvicellular. If you're searching for more information, try both spellings.)

Between each of the M and P layers lies a zone of very small cells: the interlaminar, or koniocellular (K), layers. K cells are functionally and neurochemically distinct from M and P cells and provide a third channel to the visual cortex.

The magnocellular, parvocellular, and koniocellular layers of the LGN correspond with the similarly-named types of ganglion cells.

M and P Cells

Magnocellular cells (commonly called M cells) have large cell bodies, use a relatively short time to process information, and are part of a visual processing system that tells the brain where something is. This system operates quickly but without much detail. They are found in layers 1 and 2 of the LGN, those layers more ventrally located which are next to the incoming optic tract fibers.

Parvocellular cells (commonly called P cells) have small cell bodies, use a relatively long time to process information, and are part of a visual processing system that tells the brain what something is. This system operates more slowly and with lots of information about details. For example, these cells carry color information while magnocellular cells do not. Parvocellular cells are found in layers 3, 4, 5 and 6.

Ipsilateral and Contralateral Layers

Additionally, the layers are divided up so that the eye on the same side (the ipsilateral eye) sends information to layers 2, 3 and 5 while the eye on the opposite side (the contralateral eye) sends information to layers 1, 4 and 6. (A simple mnemonic for this is that 2 + 3 = 5 while 1 + 4 does not equal 6, so it is "contra"ry to your knowledge of math.)

Remember that in visual perception, the right eye gets information from the right side of the world (the right visual field) as well as the left side of the world (the left visual field). You can confirm this by covering your left eye: the right eye still sees to your left and right, but on the left side, your vision is partially blocked by your nose.

In the LGN, the corresponding information from the right and left eyes is "stacked" so that a toothpick driven through the club sandwich of layers 1 through 6 would hit the same point in visual space six different times.
lgnlayersandmagnoparvo
Retinotopic Map
The spatial position of the ganglion cells within the retina is preserved by the spatial organisation of the neurons within the LGN layers. The posterior LGN contains neurons whose receptive field are near the fovea. Progressing from posterior to anterior, the receptive field locations become increasingly peripheral in the retina (see Erwin et al., 1999). This spatial layout is called retinotopic organization because the topological organization of the receptive fields in the LGN parallels the organization of the retina.
retinotopicmap
LGN Output

Information leaving the LGN travels out on the optic radiations, which form part of the retrolenticular limb of the internal capsule.

The axons which leave the LGN go to V1 visual cortex (areas 17 & 18)and generally end in layer IV.

Axons from layer VI of visual cortex send information back to the LGN. 
optictractpathway


Primary Visual Cortex
broadmanimg
POSTSYNAPTIC FIBERS after the lateral geniculate body fan out in the optic radiation, a tract that extends back to the occipital lobe of the cerebral hemisphere. The primary visual cortex includes Brodmann areas 17, 18, and 19. Appreciation of the image is done in a variety of association areas, particularly on the parietal and temporal lobes of the hemisphere.
 
Area 17
Area 17 essentially draws the lines and boundaries of objects in the image. Lateral geniculate output is directed to this cortex. This neuronal output is translated into simple and complex fields of cortical columns. simplecellsimgPoints of light, which can stimulate the retina very effectively, cause almost no response in visual cortex. Instead, lines of patterns representing the borders between parts of the retina responding to the distal stimulus (light) and neighboring parts of the retina that are not cause a strong contrast reaction within rectangular fields of cortical columns. This is the basis of our ability to see edges of objects and the boundaries between objects in the world.
   Cortical fields are usually depicted as a central rectangle flanked by two surround rectangles. The placement of excitation within the boundaries of these three rectangles is based on the projections from the retina/geniculate, but an expanded rule is applying--multiple centers in line are drawing the image. With simple cells, an image aligned along a row of appropriate centers yields a very strong response. Moving that line into the surround or changing the orientation (angle) of the line has a strong effect on signal; if the bar is rotated 90° into a horizontal position, the signal generated by these particular cortical fields would disappear entirely at about a 45° angle.
   Complex fields are generated in a different layer of the cortex from the simple fields. Complex fields are usually larger in perimeter than are simple fields. On/off relationships are not so well demarked, Angle of the the stimulus is not so important, but movement of a wave of excitation across a field (representing movement of the image across the retinal ganglion cells) has a very strong effect.
   The cortex of area 17 is a mixture of simple and complex fields, and their interaction draws both the outlines (including stopping points) and relative movement of visual images. Apparently in the hierarchy of evaluation, the simple fields analyze multiple cells of the eye/geniculate input, while complex fields analyze multiple simple fields.
LGNtoarea17
tuningofsimplecellsinarea17area17simplecellarrangement
Area 18
   Area 18 participates in the coloration of the drawn image of area 17, but 17 is involved with this process as well. The actual process of recognition of colors is only poorly understood and involves layers of the cortex which are organized as "blobs" outside the system of simple and complex fields. Since an on-response might be related to a green cone, while off-response cells may be cones of another color or also of green--it gets very complex! The eye is also able to correct for variation of ambient light color (such as, for example, sunset) in reconstructing the color shades of objects.
Area 19
   Area 19 of the occipital cortex is a motor association area which receives input from the lateral geniculate and many other regions. This area is aware in a geographical sense, translating the image into motor coordinates that are referred onward to the mesencephalic tectum, described above. These motor calculations track the movement of objects and also changes in position of the eyes and of the head so that the image is not blurred and is projected to the "correct" position in the outside world. But area 19 doesn't actually "see" anything.
Retina to Visual Cortex
Blindsight: Remember Area 19 of the visual cortex doesn't actually "see", but rather directs motor movements around detection of motion in subcortical parts of the visual system. There are numerous clinical cases primarily involving head injuries and strokes in which Area 17 and associated pathways are destroyed or severely damaged, but Area 19 and its pathways are still intact. This results in a fascinating type of blindness known  as "blindsight". If you held a baseball up in front of someone with this syndrome they would respond as any blind person would... they would be unable to tell you the name of the object you are holding (in fact they wouldn't even know that you were holding an object-- they are blind after all). HOWEVER.... if you proceed to toss the baseball to them...they will, almost as if by magic, raise their arm and catch the baseball!! If you then ask them to look at the object in their hand and describe its appearance.... they won't be able to! They are now "blind again"
... thus there are really TWO VISUAL PATHWAYS in the brain:
1.  PRIMARY VISUAL PATHWAY
The pathway that leads to the conscious awareness of objects in the world (our "normal" sense of vision). These pathways lead from LGN to Area 17 and Area 18 in the cortex of the brain.
2.  SECONDARY VISUAL PATHWAY
A second visual pathway which allows are body to reflexively respond to moving objects (without our even having to think about it or be aware of it) such as baseballs flying towards our face! These pathways lead from LGN to Superior Colliculus to Area 19 in the cortex of the brain.